Journal Information
Vol. 88. Issue S3.
Pages S192-S201 (November - December 2022)
Share
Share
Download PDF
More article options
Visits
2612
Vol. 88. Issue S3.
Pages S192-S201 (November - December 2022)
Review article
Full text access
Ocular findings from otoneurological examinations in children with and without dyslexia: a systematic review with meta-analysis
Visits
2612
Ysa Karen dos Santos Macambiraa,
Corresponding author
ysakaren@gmail.com

Corresponding author.
, Jessyca Vanessa dos Santos Barbosab, Bianca Manchester de Queirogab,c, Ana Augusta de Andrade Cordeirob,c, Denise Costa Menezesb,c, Maria Luiza Lopes Timóteo de Limab,c, Karina Paes Advínculab,c
a Universidade Federal de Pernambuco, Departamento de Fonoaudiologia, Recife, PE, Brazil
b Universidade Federal de Pernambuco (UFPE), Programa de Pós-Graduação em Comunicação Humana em Saúde, Recife, PE, Brazil
c Universidade Federal de Pernambuco (UFPE), Curso de Fonoaudiologia, Recife, PE, Brazil
Highlights

  • Vestibular evaluation of children with dyslexia.

  • Ocular evidence of otoneurology in dyslexia.

  • The measurements of ocular tests in children with dyslexia.

  • Vectoelectro-nystagmography and videonystagmography exams in children with dyslexia.

This item has received
Article information
Abstract
Full Text
Bibliography
Download PDF
Statistics
Figures (4)
Show moreShow less
Tables (6)
Table 1. Full texts excluded from the analysis.
Table 2. Characteristics of included studies.
Table 3. Mean and standard deviation of the amplitudes of saccades for children with and without dyslexia, per study.
Table 4. Mean and standard deviation of fixation duration for children with and without dyslexia, per study.
Table 5. Mean and standard deviations of the total number of saccades for children with and without dyslexia, per study.
Table 6. Quality of articles included, according to the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale.
Show moreShow less
Additional material (1)
Special issue
This article is part of special issue:
Vol. 88. Issue S3
More info
Abstract
Objective

To investigate ocular movements measures of vectoelectro-nystagmography and video-nystagmography in dyslexic children and compare with measures of typical children.

Methods

A systematic review of observational studies comparing the ocular movements differences between dyslexic and typical children with no publication date or language restriction. The literature survey included the bibliographic databases MEDLINE, ScienceDirect, Scopus, Web of Science, SciELO, Lilacs and CENTRAL. Gray literature databases were also searched, including: OpenGrey.eu, DissOnline.de, The New York Academy of Medicine and WorldCat. The meta-analysis was performed using software RevMan 5.3 (Cochran Collaboration).

Results

A total of 2375 articles were found of which 113 fell within the inclusion criteria. Among these, 52 were duplicates (found in more than one research source), and 45 articles were selected for reading in full. Thirteen (13) articles were included for analysis and discussion. Meta-analysis showed statistical differences between the two groups for the total number of saccades and duration of fixation.

Conclusion

The study revealed that children with dyslexia have longer duration of fixation and fewer saccades during ocular movements on vectoelectro-nystagmography and video-nystagmography when compared to children without dyslexia.

Keywords:
Dyslexia
Disorder of language
Video-nystagmography
Vectoelectro-nystagmography
Eye tests
Full Text
Introduction

Dyslexia is considered a specific neurobiological learning disorder that affects basic reading and language skills. It occurs due to differences in the functioning of brain systems responsible for phonological processing that result in difficulty in processing the sounds of words and associating them with the letters or sequences of letters that represent them. Other factors that may be associated are deficits in executive functions, difficulties in auditory and/or visual processing and psychomotor development.

It is considered a specific learning disorder because its symptoms usually affect the academic performance of students without any other alteration (neurological, sensory or motor) to justify them.1

Reading and writing processes involve complex and independents functions, in which cognitive and motor abilities are used. These functions result in a correct writing and a correct word decoding during reading.2,3 Another important ability for reading and writing is the visual-motor perception, that coordenates visual information and motor programming.4

The importance of eyes movement for the reading and writing learing processes has been studied.5–8 It is not only visual accurance that it matters, but also the visual-motor behavior characterized by sacadde alternated eyes movements, tracing and fixation.9

The eyes movements acuity directly interferes on the visual fixation of the word during reading and may be altered in people with dyslexia. Different bahavior patterns of visual movements has been found in dyslexic individuals as they read.10–12

For an accurate vision, it is necessary to stabilize the image on the retina, even during head movement. In this case, there is a compensatory movement of the eyes to the opposite direction of the cephalic movement. This compensatory movement is called the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex (VOR) and involves the integration of the vestibular system and the extraocular muscles.13

This activity can be measured by objective measures such as Video-Nystagmography (VNG) and Vectoelectro-Nystagmography (VENG), which are used for the functional evaluation of the vestibular system.7,14 Vectoelectro-nystagmography is one of the most used methods to record eye movements with greater diagnostic sensitivity. It allows the measurement of vestibulo-oculomotor function parameters, by comparing stimuli and responses. It also allows the identification of the ocular direction by recording the vestibulocochlear reflex, saccades, tracing, optokinetic and ocular fixation.15–17 Videonystagmography is a computerized system that uses infrared sensors with special glasses to record, measures and analyzes eye movements.18

The aim of this study was to investigate the measurements of ocular tests from vectoelectro-nystagmography and videonystagmography exams in children with dyslexia and to compare them with typical children.

Methods

The aim of this study was to investigate ocular movements measures of vectoelectro-nystagmography and video-nystagmography in dyslexic children and compare with measures of typical children. In order to achieve it, a systematic review of the literature on observational studies was carried out in order to answer the following guiding question: “Do dyslexic children have different results in ocular tests of VNG and VENG when compared to typical children?”. The review was structured according to the items defined by the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyzes Statement (Prisma)19 and a full protocol was published in the Prospero database (http://www.crd.york.ac.uk/PROSPERO) under the registration number CRD42018081954.

Search strategy

The strategy started finding descriptors (DECs and MeSH) and Free Terms (TL) based on the first two elements of the PICo (Population, Interest, Context) present in the title. The search terms used were: (dyslexia OR developmental reading disorder OR word blindness OR developmental dyslexia OR reading disorder OR alexia OR language disorder) AND (otoneurology OR video nystagmography OR vectoelectronystagmography OR ocular tests OR ocular evidence OR nystagmus OR eye movement OR nystagmus semi spontaneous OR optokinetic nystagmus OR pendular tracking OR saccades). The complete strategy is found in the supplementary material (Supplementary Table 1).

The searches were conducted between January and July 2018 and revised in August of the same year. The following electronic bibliographic databases were searched: MEDLINE, Cochrane Central Register of Controlled Trials (CENTRAL), Latin American and Caribbean Literature in Health Sciences (LILACS), Scientific Electronic Library Online (SciELO), ScienceDirect, Scopus, Web of Science. The search of the gray literature was conducted at: OpenGrey.eu, DissOnline.de, The New York Academy of Medicine and WorldCat. There were no manual searches made for the included articles and experts in the area were not contacted to avoid the risk of citation bias.20

Eligibility criteria

The inclusion criteria were children with dyslexia and with results of ocular tests that support VNG and VENG. The exclusion criteria were children with hearing loss, visual impairment, neurological alterations, other learning disorders, cognitive alterations and terms such as neglect dyslexia, pure alexia or hemianopia.

Repeated articles in different databases were also excluded. As a control group we considered: children with normal hearing, without any neurological problem, deficits in reading or writing, central auditory processing problems, as well as visual or cognitive problems. All submitted to ocular tests of VNG and VENG under similar conditions of the test group procedure. Finally, studies with at least a title and/or abstract in English were included, but there was no other restriction on language or date of publication.

Extraction of data

This review was performed by two researchers who independently identified titles and abstracts extracted from electronic database sources that met the inclusion criteria. A third researcher would decide whenever there was a divergence between the two researchers, however, this was not needed as there was no divergence. Full texts of these potentially selected studies were entirety analyzed. The searched studies outcomes were differences in the mean values of saccadic amplitudes, duration of fixation and total number of saccades recorded by VNG and/or VENG in both group of children, associated to the specific complaint of the language disorder.

Data were analyzed from published articles and authors were contacted for additional information. In addition to the outcome data, the authors' names, title, year of publication, country, age groups of the groups and the number of subjects in each group were also extracted. A standard form for data storage was created based on the model adopted by Cochrane.21

Bias risk assessment method

The risk of bias was assessed according to the recommendations of the handbook and the Newcastle-Ottawa scale, adapted for cross-sectional observational studies. The quality of the work was also evaluated independently by two researchers and the divergences were evaluated by consensus. The list of evaluated items used for scoring was: (1) Representativeness of the sample; (2) Sample size; (3) Management of non-responses; (4) Exposure calculation (risk factor); (5) Comparability, to investigate whether individuals in different groups of outcomes are comparable, based on study design or analysis, control of confounding factors; (6) Evaluation of results and (7) Statistical testing (Supplementary Table 2). The maximum score possible was 10 points.

Data analysis method

The amplitude of the saccades, the duration of fixation and the total number of saccades measured in the ocular movements of the two groups were compared by meta-analysis. We used the effect of the mean difference between the groups as a measure and a model of random effects as a statistical method of analysis. A value of α less than 0.05 was considered statistically significant. When it was not possible to obtain adequate data for analysis, Cochrane’s recommendations were followed. Statistical heterogeneity between the studies was assessed using Cochrane's Q test and the presence of inconsistency was assessed with the I2 test A p-value < 0.10 was considered statistically significant. When necessary, study characteristics, considered to be potential sources of heterogeneity, were included in a subgroup analysis. In addition, in the case of heterogeneity, the studies were removed one by one to investigate whether a study was the source of heterogeneity. All analyses were conducted with RevMan 5.3 software (Cochrane Collaboration).21

ResultsIncluded studies

A flow diagram that illustrates the search and selection process is presented in Fig. 1. Of the 2375 titles considered relevant, 113 abstracts were selected to be read, and of these, 45 complete texts were selected for reading in full. After reading, 32 articles were excluded because they did not meet the eligibility criteria, five because they did not have enough data (Table 1). Therefore, 13 complete texts were included in the qualitative analysis and, of these, only 8 for quantitative meta-analysis (Table 2). The mean values of the saccade amplitudes, the duration of fixation and total number of saccades in children with and without dyslexia extracted from the selected articles are shown in Fig. 2 (Table 3), Fig. 3 (Table 4) and Fig. 4 (Table 5) respectively.

Figure 1.

Flowchart showing processes used to search for and select articles.

(0.2MB).
Table 1.

Full texts excluded from the analysis.

Name  Location  Year  Reason  Name  Location  Year  Reason 
Benfatto et al.  Sweden  2016  Did not analyze saccades  Bucci et al.27,a  France  2008  Insufficient data for meta-analysis 
Krieber et al.  Austria  2016  Age group  Huang et al.  China  2008  Reviewed abstract only 
Lukasova et al.24,a  Brazil  2016  Insufficient data for meta-analysis  Kirkby et al.  UK  2008  Review article 
Huettig e Brouwer  UK  2015  Age group  Judge et al.  England  2007  Age group 
Kunert e Sheepers  Scotland  2015  Did not analyze saccades  Wright  Australia  2007  Visual training 
Kim  China  2014  Reviewed abstract only  Judge et al.  England  2006  Age group 
Pan  China  2014  Did not analyze saccades  Santos et al.13,a  Brazil  1995  Insufficient data for meta-analysis 
Bucci et al.25,a  France  2014  Insufficient data for meta-analysis  Rodger  Australia  1985  Review article 
Xiu et al.  China  2013  Unsupported method  Brown et al.  USA  1983  Visual Evaluation 
Pensiero et al.  Italy  2013  Age group  Olson et al.  Germany  1983  Unsupported method 
Bellocchi e al.  France  2013  Review article  Romero et al.  USA  1982  Unsupported method 
Oliveira et al.  Brazil  2013  Age group  Pavlidis  USA  1981  Visual Training 
Kraljevic e Palmović26,a  Croatia  2011  Insufficient data for meta-analysis  Gabersek et al.  France  1981  Unsupported method 
Quercia  France  2010  Review article  Elterman et al.  Canada  1980  Reviewed abstract only 
Hawelka et al.  Germany  2010  Unsupported method  Leisman e Schwartz  USA  1978  Unsupported method 
Medland et al.  UK  2010  Visual Evaluation  Adler-Grinberg e Stark  USA  1978  Visual training 
Thaler et al.  Germany  2009  Population with comorbidity  Bogacz et al.  USA  1974  Age group 
Kapoula et al.  France  2009  Reviewed abstract only  Zangwill e Blakemore  USA  1972  Unsupported method 
Kirk  UK  2009  Visual Training         
a

Articles excluded only from meta-analysis but included in the qualitative evaluation.

Table 2.

Characteristics of included studies.

Authors  Year  Local  Apparatus/Method  N of the dyslexic group  N of the control group 
Tiadi et al.24  2016  France  Video-oculografia (EyeBrainr T2).  55 (IM = 10.2)  55 (AA = 8.34)a e 55 (AA = 10.5)b 
Vagge et al.25  2015  Switzerland  Video-oculografia infravermelha (OBER 2 System)  11 (IM = 9.4)  11 (AA = 9.2)b 
Seassau et al.26  2014  France  Video-oculografia  43 (IM = 10.6)  42 (AA = 10.7)a 
Tiadi et al.27  2014  France  Video-oculografia infravervelha (mobileEBT®, e(ye)BRAIN)  56 (IM = 10.5)  56 (AA = 10.6)b 
Pan et al.28  2013  China  Video-oculografia (EyeLink II – Eye Tracker de alta velocidade)  30 (IM = 10.7)  26 (AA = 10.6)b 
Bucci et al.29  2012  USA  Video-oculografia infravervelha (mobileEBTH, e(ye)BRAIN).  12 (IM = 11)  10 (AA = 8.3)a e 9 (AA = 11)b 
Jainta e Kapoula30  2011  USA  Video-oculografia (Chronos Eye-tracking).  13 (IM = 11.7)  7 (AA = 12.7)b 
Bucci et al.31  2007  France  Video-oculografia fotoelétrica (Oculomotor-Bouis).  18 (IM = 11.4)  13 (AA = 11.1)b 

AA, Average age.

a

Group paired for school age.

b

Group paired by chronological age.

Figure 2.

Meta-analysis: comparison of means and standard deviations of the amplitudes of saccades, for dyslexic children and for non-dyslexic children, per study.

(0.16MB).
Table 3.

Mean and standard deviation of the amplitudes of saccades for children with and without dyslexia, per study.

Authors  Average amplitude of saccades (±SD)
  Dyslexic group  Control group 
Bucci et al.31  27.1 ± 1  35 ± 0.8 
Bucci et al.26  29.1 ± 1.8  36.9 ± 1.8 
Jainta e Kapoula30  2.42 ± 0.99  2.09 ± 0.52 
Tiadi et al.24  8.6 ± 1.3  9.7 ± 1.2 
Figure 3.

Meta-analysis: comparison of means and standard deviations of the durations of saccadi fixations, for dyslexic children and for non-dyslexic children, per study.

(0.15MB).
Table 4.

Mean and standard deviation of fixation duration for children with and without dyslexia, per study.

Authors  Average length of saccades fixation (±SD) ms
  Dyslexic group  Control group 
Bucci et al.31  1497 ± 107  337 ± 29 
Jainta e Kapoula30  351 ± 118.4  280.1 ± 85.1 
Pan et al.28  322 ± 42  283 ± 30 
Tiadi et al.27  291.2 ± 36.4  235.5 ± 34.3 
Figure 4.

Meta-analysis: comparison of means and standard deviations of the total number of saccades, for dyslexic children and for non-dyslexic children, per study.

(0.13MB).
Table 5.

Mean and standard deviations of the total number of saccades for children with and without dyslexia, per study.

Authors  Average number of total saccades (±SD)
  Dyslexic group  Control group 
Bucci et al.29  95 ± 9  36 ± 3 
Vagge et al.25  170.8 ± 77.7  101.9 ± 32.7 
Risk assessment of bias

The analysis of the quality of the included articles22–34 and the risk of bias associated with disease (Table 1) are shown in Table 6.

Table 6.

Quality of articles included, according to the Newcastle-Ottawa quality assessment scale.

Authors  Representativeness of the sample  Justified sample size  Rate of non-answers  Exposure calculation  Comparability  Evaluation of results  Appropriate statistical testing  Final evaluation 
Tiadi, Gérard, Peyre et al. (2016)24  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Lukasova, Silva and Macedo (2016)20  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Vagge, Cavanna, Traverso et al. (2015)25  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 
Bucci, Mélithe, Ajrezo et al.(2014)21  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Seassau, Gérard, Bui-Quoc et al. (2014)26  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Tiadi, Seassau, Bui-Quoc et al. (2014)27  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Pan, Yan, Laubrock, Hua Shu et al. (2013)28  Not representative  Yes  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  7/10 
Bucci, Nassibi, Gerard et al. (2012)29  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 
Kraljevic and Palmovic (2011)22  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  5/10 
Jainta and Kapoula (2011)30  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 
Bucci, Gignac and Kapoula (2008)23  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 
Bucci, Gignac and Kapoula (2007)31  Not representativa  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 
Santos, Behlau and Caovilla (1995)12  Not representative  No  Validated tool  Yes  Own report  Yes  6/10 

Result presented in the form: points obtained/maximum score.

aMinimum criterion of n ≥ 30 (central limit theorem).

bMaximum score of 10 stars.

All studies included were characterized as observational and cross-sectional studies. In addition, at the final evaluation, all of them had a percentage of quality equal or superior to 50% (5/10) and six obtained a maximum score of 70% (7/10). The sample size of children with dyslexia was a concern for six studies28–31,33,34 that fit the central limit theorem, with samples equal to or greater than 30 subjects. However, none of them had performed calculations to estimate the minimum size of their samples. All studies population were selected by convenience. The non-response rate was not described on the studies. All used tools for data collection were validated and comparability between the control group and the dyslexic group was also possible for all of them. The evaluation of the results was done in all the works by means of own report. Finally, all studies used appropriate statistical tests.

Data analysis

As the studies were not randomized, the groups presented great discrepancy at the first evaluation. Thus, to avoid the phenomenon of regression to the mean, it was necessary to analyse the variations between the final and initial values of the amplitudes, total number and duration of fixation of the saccades, as well as the standard deviation associated to these variations.

Amplitude of saccades

For the comparison of the amplitude of saccades, 4 articles (described in Table 3) were meta-analysed. The mean difference of this component for dyslexic amplitude was -3.33, with 95% CI (-6.82–0.15). The overall effect test yielded p < 0.06 and revealed that such a difference was not significant. The value found for the heterogeneity was I2 = 98%, with p < 0.00001 (Fig. 2).

Fixation time

Four articles were submitted to metanalyses for fixation duration (described in Table 4). For this parameter, the mean interval for dyslexics was 1.19 ms with 95% CI (0.68–1.70). The overall effect test yielded p < 0.0001 and revealed that such difference was significant. However, a heterogeneity value I2 = 53% was found, with p < 0.12 (Fig. 3).

Total number saccades

The number of articles to be meta-analysed for the total number of saccades was only 2 (described in Table 5). The mean difference of this component for the number of saccades was 59.12, with 95% CI (53.69–64.54). The overall effect test yielded p < 0.000001 and revealed significant difference. In addition, the heterogeneity was I2 = 0%, with p < 0.70 (Fig. 4), considered perfect.

Discussion

Approximately half of the analyzed studies shows immaturity of the cortical areas that control the visual fixation system in dyslexic children when compared to children with typical development.29,32,34 In dyslexic people, the percentage with poor binocular coordination during and after the saccades, is greater than in control cases. In this way, listening to the teacher in the classroom, copying and transcribing written lessons on the blackboard, reading the books lessons, writing and concentrating become complex activities for dyslexics since they lack the integrity of oculomotor functions and vestibular interconnections.18,35–37 A clear vision requires a quick alignment of the fovea with the object of interest (saccade) and keep it aligned to this object for a sufficient period of time so that the visual system can perform a detailed analysis of the image (fixation duration). In general, saccadic movements are interesting due to their close relation with attention. Its measurement may reveal disturbances in oculomotor activation and help diagnosis of neurological disorders.14

Data from the selected articles confirms the existence of a considerable difference in the ocular motor parameters between the groups. This disparate behaviour has been described in a study31 of 112 children with a mean age of 10.4 years, divided into two groups: 56 typical children and 56 with dyslexia. The authors compared latency values and found an increase in the dyslexic group. Similar results were found, in another French study24 of 30 children, with a mean age of 11.6 years, also divided into dyslexic (16) and non-dyslexic (14).

In addition, measurements of gain and mean velocities were reduced in the dyslexic groups with the occurrence of anticipatory saccades and large numbers of long-lasting, unstable fixations compared with the control group (Kraljević and Palmović 2011; Bucci et al., 2012; Bucci et al., 2004; Seassau et al., 2014). Several hypotheses were raised in order to justify such a difference: reduction or deficiency in the processing of visual attention and the precision of the search; immaturity of the saccadic ocular interaction and vergence systems; and the possibility of visual/ocular motor imperfections were frequently cited.

The impairment in convergence, ocular saccadic interaction, and fusion capacity of divergence may be due to a lack of cortical maturation, and the poor oculomotor performance in children with dyslexia is possibly related to a deficit in the allocation of visual attention, suggesting a processing deficiency.22–24,28,33

Learning disability, related to dyslexia, can still be justified by the interaction between the saccadic and vergence subsystems, on which knowledge acquisition may be based.27

VNG is a computerized system that uses the principle of capturing eye movements by infrared sensors placed on special glasses or mask. The movements are measured and analyzed on a video monitor and further recorded. In VENG, the most commonly used method for recording ocular movements, the variation of corneal-retinal potential, is used to record and analyze features of the vestibulo-ocular reflex and saccadic visual systems involved in the persecution, optokinesis and fixation.18,36

The use of VNG and VENG exams has already been consolidated in several pathologies that involve the cerebral cortex, the brainstem, the superior colliculus, the basal ganglia, the cerebellum and the sensory organs (semicircular canals and otolithic organs). This neural system controls the ocular fixation in various conditions such as the movement of the visual object, movement of the observer and changes in vision. Among the most common pathologies associated with this network are: petrositis (infection in the petrous part of the temporal bone that can reach the fourth cranial nerves, multiple sclerosis (demyelinating immune disease), optic neuritis (visual or subacute visual loss (vogt-koyanagi-harada syndrome, an autoimmune disease affecting eyes, skin, ears, and meninges) and congenital rubella (viral infection by vertical transmission, which mainly affects vision, hearing and the cardiac function of a newborn). Scientific and clinical evidence should be reasons to extend the use of these exams to the differential diagnosis on learning disorders, such as dyslexia, which does not yet have specific neurobiological cause described in the literature.13,38–41

When speaking of otoneurological evaluation we usually refer to practices performed by the speech-language pathologist related to the investigation of auditory and vestibular function, with the objective of assisting in the diagnosis of the disturbances of balance. However, the range of possibilities offered by the instruments such as VENG to the researcher is immense, especially from the perspective of making the diagnosis of language disorders more accurate, as in the case of dyslexia.42

It turns out, however, that the qualitative analysis of the studies of this review have not showed data or discussion about vestibular complaints in dyslexic population. This must be due to the lack of research using VNG and VENG for these purposes. These tests are fundamentally recommended to people with vestibular problems, and it is important to reinforce the need for more studies.

Conclusion

The study revealed that children with dyslexia have longer duration of fixation and fewer saccades during ocular movements on vectoelectro-nystagmography and video-nystagmography when compared to children without dyslexia. However, all these findings are not intended to reduce dyslexia to a purely ocular disorder, since, as noted above, it is a complex disorder that may be associated with deficits in executive functions, difficulties in auditory and/or visual processing and psychomotor development.1

Conflicts of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

Appendix A
Supplementary data

The following is Supplementary data to this article:

References
[1]
American Psychiatric Association.
Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders.
5th ed., American Psychiatric Publishing, (2013),
[2]
T.E. Johnson, T.N. Archibald, G. Tenenbaum.
Individual and team annotation effects on students’ reading comprehension, critical thinking, and meta-cognitive skills.
Comput Human Behav, 26 (2010), pp. 1496-1507
[3]
F. Rosa Neto, E.R. Santos, J. Toro.
Manual de Desempenho Escolar: Análise da leitura e escrita: Series iniciais do Ensino Fundamental.
Unisul, (2010), pp. 1-121
[4]
G.D. Germano, F.H. Pinheiro, P.M.M. Okuda, S.A. Capellini.
Percepção viso-motora de escolares com Transtorno do Déficit de Atenção com Hiperatividade.
[5]
D.F.P. Ventura, L. Ganato, E.I. Mitre, R. Mor.
Parâmetros de oculomotricidade à nistagmografia digital em crianças com e sem distúrbios de aprendizagem.
Braz J Otorhinolaryngol, 75 (2009), pp. 733-737
[6]
P.G. Mathes, C.A. Denton.
The prevention and identification of reading disability.
Semin Pediatr Neurol, 9 (2002), pp. 185-191
[7]
C. Gaertner, M.P. Bucci, L. Ajrezo, S. Wiener-Vacher.
Binocular coordination of saccades during reading in children with clinically assessed poor vergence capabilities.
Vision Res, 87 (2013), pp. 22-29
[8]
S. Pensiero, A. Accardo, P. Michieletto, P. Brambilla.
Saccadic alterations in severe developmental dyslexia.
Case Rep Neurol Med, 2013 (2013), pp. 406-861
[9]
M. Valsecchi, K.R. Gegenfurtner, A.C. Schütz.
Saccadic and smooth-pursuit eye movements during reading of drifting texts.
J Vision, 13 (2013), pp. 8
[10]
R.D. Elterman, L.A. Abel, R.B. Daroff, L.F. Dell’Osso, J.L. Bornstein.
Eye movement patterns in dyslexic children.
J Learn Disabil, 13 (1980), pp. 16-21
[11]
M. Biscaldi, S. Gezeck, V. Stuhr.
Poor saccadic control correlates with dyslexia.
Neuropsychologia, 36 (1998), pp. 1189-1202
[12]
F. Hutzler, H. Wimmer.
Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography.
Brain Lang, 89 (2004), pp. 235-242
[13]
R. Sales, J.F. Colafêmina.
A influência da oculomotricidade e do reflexo-vestíbulo-ocular na leitura e escrita.
Rev CEFAC, 16 (2014), pp. 1791-1797
[14]
F.C.Z. Maia, P.L.M. Albernaz, S. Carmona.
Otoneurologia Atual.
Revinter, (2014), pp. 53-102
[15]
H.H. Caovilla, M.M. Ganança, M.S.L. Munhoz, M.L.G. Silva, M.M. Frazza.
O valor da nistagmografia computadorizada.
Rev Bras Med Otorrinolaringol, 4 (1997), pp. 158-163
[16]
M. Soriano-Ferrer.
Implicaciones educativas del deficit cognitivo de la dislexia evolutiva [Educational implications of the cognitive impairment in developmental dyslexia].
Rev Neurol, 38 Suppl 1 (2004), pp. S47-S52
[17]
R. Mor, M. Fragoso.
Vestibulometria na prática fonoaudiológica.
Pulso Editorial, (2012), pp. 1-104
[18]
M.M. Ganança, H.H. Caovilla, F.F. Ganança.
Eletronistagmografia versus videonistagmografia.
Braz J Otorhinolaryngol, 76 (2010), pp. 399-403
[19]
D. Moher, A. Liberati, J. Tetzlaff, D.G. Altman, T.P. Group.
Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement.
PLoS Med, 6 (2009),
[20]
J.A.C. Sterne, M. Egger, D. Moher.
Addressing reporting biases.
Cochran handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, pp. 297-325
[21]
J.P.T. Higgins, D.G. Altman, J.A.C. Sterne.
Assessing risk of bias in included studies.
Cochran handbook for systematic reviews of interventions, pp. 8-73
[22]
M.T.M. Santos, M.S. Behlau, H.H. Caovilla.
Crianças com distúrbios de leitura e escrita: movimentos oculares na leitura à nistagmografia computadorizada.
Rev Bras Med Otorrinolaringol, 2 (1995), pp. 100-108
[23]
M.P. Bucci, D. Brémond-Gignac, Z. Kapoula.
Poor binocular coordination of saccades in dyslexic children.
Graefe’s Arch Clin Exp Ophthalmol, 246 (2008), pp. 417-428
[24]
M.P. Bucci, D. Brémond-Gignac, Z. Kapoula.
Latency of saccades and vergence eye movements in dyslexic children.
Exp Brain Res, 188 (2008), pp. 1-12
[25]
S. Jainta, Z. Kapoula.
Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading.
PLoS One, 6 (2011),
[26]
J.K. Kraljević, M. Palmović.
Spatial and temporal measurements of eye movement in children with dyslexia.
Coll Antropol, 35 (2011), pp. 191-198
[27]
M.P. Bucci, N. Nassibi, C.L. Gerard, E. Bui-Quoc, M. Seassau.
Immaturity of the oculomotor saccade and vergence interaction in dyslexic children: evidence from a reading and visual search study.
PLoS One, 7 (2012),
[28]
J. Pan, M. Yan, J. Laubrock, H. Shu, R. Kliegl.
Eye-voice span during rapid automatized naming of digits and dice in Chinese normal and dyslexic children.
Dev Sci, 16 (2013), pp. 967-979
[29]
M.P. Bucci, D. Mélithe, L. Ajrezo, E. Bui-Quoc, C.L. Gérard.
The influence of oculomotor tasks on postural control in dyslexic children.
Front Hum Neurosci, 8 (2014), pp. 981
[30]
M. Seassau, C.L. Gérard, E. Bui-Quoc, M.P. Bucci.
Binocular saccade coordination in reading and visual search: a developmental study in typical reader and dyslexic children.
Front Hum Neurosci, 8 (2014), pp. 85
[31]
A. Tiadi, M. Seassau, E. Bui-Quoc, C.L. Gerard, M.P. Bucci.
Vertical saccades in dyslexic children.
Res Dev Disabil, 35 (2014), pp. 3175-3181
[32]
A. Vagge, M. Cavanna, C.E. Traverso, M. Iester.
Evaluation of ocular movements in patients with dyslexia.
Ann Dyslexia, 65 (2015), pp. 24-32
[33]
K. Lukasova, I.P. Silva, E.C. Macedo.
Impaired oculomotor behavior of children with developmental dyslexia in antisaccades and predictive saccades tasks.
Front Psychol, 7 (2016), pp. 987
[34]
A. Tiadi, C.L. Gérard, H. Peyre, E. Bui-Quoc, M.P. Bucci.
Immaturity of visual fixations in dyslexic children.
Front Hum Neurosci, 10 (2016), pp. 58
[35]
E.S. Franco, E.B. Caetanelli.
Analysis of the vestibular system in children without hearing and vestibular complaints by computerized vectonystagmography.
Int Arch Otorhinolaryngol, 10 (2006), pp. 46-54
[36]
C.A. Moreira.
Relações entre o Aparelho Visual e o Sistema Vestibulococlear.
Multidisciplinaridade na Otoneurologia, pp. 1-512
[37]
D.P. Vaz, S.M. Lança, R.C. Scharlach, M.M. Ganança, J.M. Gazzola.
Doença de Corpúsculos de Lewy: um relato de caso.
Rev Equilíbrio Corpor Saúde, 2 (2015), pp. 60-75
[38]
H.O. Barber, C.W. Stockwell.
Manual of electronystagmography.
C.V. Mosby, (1980), pp. 230
[39]
M.S. Starr, K. Rayner.
Eye movements during reading: Some current controversies.
Trends Cogn Sci, 4 (2001), pp. 156-163
[40]
S. Pensiero, A. Accardo, P. Michieletto, P. Brambilla.
Saccadic alterations in severe developmental dyslexia.
Case Rep Neurol Med, 2013 (2013),
[41]
D.M. Lourenço, I.M. Buscatti, B. Lourenço, F.C. Monti, J.A. Paz, C.A. Silva.
Neurite óptica em paciente com artrite idiopática juvenil.
Rev Bras Reumatol, 54 (2014), pp. 486-489
[42]
Resolução CFFa Nº 384. Lei nº 6.965/81. Decreto nº 87.218/82. competência técnica e legal do fonoaudiólogo para realizar avaliação vestibular e terapia fonoaudiológica em equilíbrio/reabilitação vestibular. [Sessão Plenária Ordinária Web site]. April 19, 2010. Available at: https://www.legisweb.com.br/legislacao/?id=112946. Accessed January 01, 2021.

Peer Review under the responsibility of Associação Brasileira de Otorrinolaringologia e Cirurgia Cérvico-Facial.

Copyright © 2022. Associação Brasileira de Otorrinolaringologia e Cirurgia Cérvico-Facial
Idiomas
Brazilian Journal of Otorhinolaryngology (English Edition)
Article options
Tools
Supplemental materials
en pt
Announcement Nota importante
Articles submitted as of May 1, 2022, which are accepted for publication will be subject to a fee (Article Publishing Charge, APC) payment by the author or research funder to cover the costs associated with publication. By submitting the manuscript to this journal, the authors agree to these terms. All manuscripts must be submitted in English.. Os artigos submetidos a partir de 1º de maio de 2022, que forem aceitos para publicação estarão sujeitos a uma taxa (Article Publishing Charge, APC) a ser paga pelo autor para cobrir os custos associados à publicação. Ao submeterem o manuscrito a esta revista, os autores concordam com esses termos. Todos os manuscritos devem ser submetidos em inglês.